Leadership

3 biased reasons why quality is not like porn

Former United States Supreme Court Justice, Potter Stewart, said that while pornography may be insurmountably difficult to define, “I know it when I see it.”

The Norwegian Ministry of Education and Research is currently writing a white paper about quality in higher education and in that context Minister Torbjørn Røe Isaksen has been piggybacking on Stewart’s quote, stating that “quality is like porn: it’s difficult to define, but I know it when I see it.”

This is a dubious claim. In particular, the idea that one knows quality when one sees it flies in the face of both real-life experience and research. Consider evidence from three domains.

Evaluating students

In Norway, final exams are marked by a commission of at least two faculty members. Students who are dissatisfied with their grades can appeal and have their papers remarked by a new commission. In such cases, the new commissions are not made aware of the original grade.

The Ministry has expressed its dissatisfaction with this system, after several high-profile cases in which students received  substantially different grades from the two commissions. At least two students originally given a B were given an F the second time around, while two others originally were given Fs but then the new commission upgraded them to As! The system for appeals, the Ministry claimed, was not working as intended.

“There is just one perfect child, and every mother has it.”

It may not be working as intended, but it certainly is working as anyone familiar with the research could have predicted. Professors carrying out the grading — all of whom are experts in the relevant area — just do not agree on the quality of exam papers. Whether the paper is good or bad is not something that simply must be seen and recognized; experts vary in their opinions about quality. Research on the Norwegian grading system is both clear and disturbing on exactly this point. In one example, the rate of agreement on whether an exam should pass or fail was about at chance. Grading is not objective and the quality of an exam is not simply a matter of acknowledging the obvious, i.e. of knowing it when you see it.

Evaluating colleagues

Peer review is well-established internationally as the hallmark of quality control in science. When we think our work is ready to share with our colleagues, we submit an article to an academic journal where editors determine whether or not it should be published in part by administering a peer review process. The submitted article is sent out to internationally leading experts who evaluate it and report back to the editors with their frankest assessment of the quality of the piece.

I would wager that every researcher who has ever submitted an article to a serious journal has had the experience of receiving conflicting reviews — one reviewer may conclude that the paper is almost good enough to be published (because it’s never completely good enough!) and a second may say it’s nowhere close. Indeed, as a former editor of one of the leading journals in my field, I’m sure I am not alone in claiming that a core editorial task is to determine how to weigh or reconcile radically divergent reviews.

These reviews, as noted, are carried out by leading researchers in the relevant field. Why don’t they agree? Quality just isn’t obvious; how one weighs different factors leads to different assessments.

Thinking, fast and slow

The research on grading and peer review should not be surprising. Nobel laureate Daniel Kahneman built his career researching the topic of bias, which he wonderfully presents in his book Thinking, Fast and Slow. Radically oversimplifying, his research shows us that decisions and assessments are always made under the influence of context and accumulated experience. This is bias. It affects everything we do.

Grades assigned to papers are influenced by the quality of the papers read just before it.  The same is true of our evaluation of grant applications. Manuscripts of scholarly articles are judged in light of our own views of the subject and perhaps our own discoveries. This is natural; it’s what humans do. We are simply not capable of objectivity. And if the idea behind “I know it when I see it” is that subjective evaluations all will point in the same direction, well, that is a misunderstanding of what subjective means.

Research on bias is nearly as ubiquitous as bias itself. The claim that determinations of quality are exempt from bias, rejects the conclusions of a substantial research literature. Yet this is precisely what a slogan like “I know it when I see it” does.

The perfect child

While it may be difficult to define quality, a substantial body of research makes it clear that we don’t categorically recognize it when we see it. Indeed, experts routinely show significant variation in the level of quality they attribute to a particular work. If the goal of the coming white paper on quality is to define quality and then to use that definition to manage institutions, the research I’ve pointed to here must be considered.

When it comes to the ways we educate our students, there’s no reason to anticipate consensus. Indeed, the suggestion that we all will agree that someone else’s educational program is of high quality — perhaps even higher than our own — reminds me of the oft-cited proverb “There is just one perfect child, and every mother has it.”

This essay was first published in Khrono.

See also You don’t know quality when you see it: Bias and bibliometrics

My interest in moving universities towards balance encompasses gender equality, the communication of scientific results, promoting research-based education and leadership development more generally. Read more

Share

No Comments

[fbcomments width="456" count="on" num="3" countmsg="comments"]

Republish

I encourage you to republish this article online and in print, under the following conditions.

  • You have to credit the author.
  • If you’re republishing online, you must use our page view counter and link to its appearance here (included in the bottom of the HTML code), and include links from the story. In short, this means you should grab the html code below the post and use all of it.
  • Unless otherwise noted, all my pieces here have a Creative Commons Attribution licence -- CC BY 4.0 -- and you must follow the (extremely minimal) conditions of that license.
  • Keeping all this in mind, please take this work and spread it wherever it suits you to do so!